previous     table of contents     next


Section I

Introduction

The study of the learning of proof in schools is a significant field in current research in mathematics education.  The field has progressed from studies indicating that students have difficulty with proof (Senk, 1985; Fischbein, 1982) to studies of small groups engaged in proving (Reid, 1995a; de Villiers, 1992), philosophical considerations (Hanna, 1983) and ethnographies of current classroom practice (Schoenfeld, 1988;  Sekiguchi, 1991).  The study of the learning of proof takes place within a larger field of enquiry into reasoning, which has lately received increasing emphasis in mathematics education research (e.g., Stiff, 1999) This report describes the result of one phase of the Psychology of Reasoning In School Mathematics (PRISM)* project, in which a teaching experiment model was used to add to our understanding of students’ use of the reasoning that underlies proof.

The PRISM Project

The main objective of the PRISM project is to continue the development of a model (first described in Reid, 1995a) for describing students’ reasoning that includes needs.  This model is intended to be applicable to the description of mathematical reasoning at every level of schooling.  It distinguishes among individuals’ degrees of formulation of deductive reasoning, styles of reasoning, and needs related to reasoning (especially explaining, exploring and verifying).  “Needs” in this model describes the student’s goals or purposes in reasoning in a particular way and context.  Deductive reasoning has been identified by the

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-1

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM, 1989), the Cockcroft Report (1982) and other calls for reform in mathematics teaching as an area of central importance to mathematics instruction. Research in this area contributes directly to the psychology of mathematics education, and has implications for the current reform of mathematics teaching and learning in both primary and secondary schools.

Three specific questions are being investigated in the PRISM project:

1.  In what ways can the mathematical reasoning of primary school students and the needs associated with their reasoning differ from those of secondary school students?

2.  How can teaching interventions and other features of learning mathematics in schools foster emotional orientations toward reasoning and create occasions for deductive reasoning and the formulating of that reasoning?

3.  To what extent do the models of needs to reason developed in previous research describe students’ reasoning at various school levels, and how might they be improved?

The first question addresses the need for research that bridges the divide between research done with young students and research at the secondary and post-secondary level.  Work at the primary level has shown that young children can reason in sophisticated ways (English, 1996; Zack, 1995, 1997; Graves & Zack, 1996; Maher & Martino, 1996; Lampert 1990).  Research on deductive reasoning at the secondary level, however, suggests that students are not able to 

 

* This research project has been funded by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada grant #410-98-0085, and by internal grants from Memorial University and Acadia University.

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-2

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


apply their reasoning when called to do so in formal mathematical contexts (Fischbein, 1982; Senk, 1985).  In the PRISM project the same model for reasoning is being used to describe the reasoning of students at a wide range of grade levels allowing comparisons to be made that may provide insight into the development of mathematical reasoning from primary to secondary school.  The research reported here provides background for future research into this question, but does not address it directly.

The second question addresses the extent to which it is possible to create occasions for deductive reasoning while working within the constraints of existing provincial curricula and the aims of reform documents, such as the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards and Professional Standards for Teaching Mathematics (NCTM 1989, 1991).  This question is ultimately tied to a long term goal of offering researchers and teachers examples of teaching in which deductive reasoning was encouraged, and providing materials that will assist them in fostering such occasions in their own classrooms.  In the grade 2 contexts games were used as the primary contexts for study.  Result relevant to this question will be discussed in Section III.

The third question is concerned with the feasibility of understanding the deductive reasoning of younger students using existing models.  Can characteristics of the development of deductive reasoning in primary school students be described using models for describing the reasoning of secondary school students?  Investigations of this question will produce improved models for examining mathematical reasoning at all levels.   The research reported here addresses the reasoning of younger students, and modifications to the model resulting from this research will be discussed in Section III.

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-3

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


 

Context

Previous research on deductive reasoning.

Research into deductive reasoning in mathematics has been conducted from several different perspectives.  The considerable body of research into logic and mathematical proof, conducted especially in connection with the New Math reforms, contains much that is relevant.   A smaller body of research (Reid, 1995a; de Villiers, 1992; Bell, 1976) exists that is concerned directly with the nature of deductive reasoning in mathematics, and the needs that reasoning can satisfy.  There also exist several research projects directly concerned with mathematical reasoning in school contexts.

The main conclusion of previous research on deductive reasoning is that most students do not understand the nature of proof (e.g., Fischbein, 1982; Senk, 1985) and do not reason logically in many situations (Henle, 1962; Wason, 1966).  Large scale studies in the United States have found that most students (e.g., 70% in Senk, 1985) do not understand the proofs they study in high school geometry.  Other studies have indicated that students are confused about the relationship between examples and proofs (Alibert, 1988; Bell, 1976; Chazan, 1993; de Villiers, 1992; Finlow-Bates, 1994; Fischbein, 1982).   Other researchers (Balacheff, 1991; Braconne & Dionne, 1987; Schoenfeld, 1988; Shanny & Erlich, 1992) have found that both students and teachers concentrate much more on the appearance of proofs than on their logical content.  This focus contributes to problems in understanding proof (Schoenfeld, 1988).

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-4

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


Most of the research on proof mentioned above does not consider students’ emotional orientations to be important to their reasoning.  There is an implicit assumption that the need of many mathematicians to use deductive reasoning to verify mathematical statements is shared by secondary school students. Research by Reid (1995a), building on the psychological studies of de Villiers (1991) and Bell (1976), indicates that students often use deductive reasoning to explore and explain mathematical phenomena, suggesting that an emotional orientation to satisfy a need to explain or explore through deductive reasoning is already present in some secondary students.  The assumption that students have an emotional orientation toward using deductive reasoning to satisfy a need to verify may be at the root of students’ poor understanding of proofs and other topics related to deductive reasoning.  Work in the philosophy and sociology of mathematics by Hanna (1983), Tymoczko (1986), Lakatos (1976), and others, shows that the use of deduction to explore and explain is also widespread among professional mathematicians, suggesting that the concept of mathematical proof at the post-secondary level could be developed from the emotional orientations secondary school students already have.

The PRISM project is be a continuation of a program of research into students’ reasoning processes begun by Reid in 1990.  Initial research into university students’ understanding of proof by mathematical induction (Reid, 1992) revealed the importance of formulation in students’ reasoning, especially in the development of informal or everyday reasoning into mathematical proving (Reid, 1993).  It also suggested that the needs students had for reasoning deductively had significant implications for the development of mathematical reasoning.  More recently a study of the needs deductive reasoning satisfies for high school and university students and the contexts in which these needs arise has 

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-5

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


revealed the importance of exploring and explaining as needs for reasoning deductively (Reid 1994a, b, 1995a, b, c).  This research into the psychology of mathematical reasoning has provided a basis for developing the PRISM project, which seeks to investigate whether these results are applicable in schools.

The Model

The model developed in earlier research addresses four aspects of reasoning: needs; types; formulation; and formality.  In the PRISM project, special attention is being paid to elaborating on needs and formulation.

Needs are the purposes or goals of reasoning.  Explaining, exploring and verification are the main needs identified in the current model. 

Types of reasoning covered are: deductive (reasoning from general principles); inductive (the generalization of specific cases); and analogical (reasoning by similarities).  These three types are suggested by Pólya (1968).  Two other types, abductive reasoning and generalizing, have been added as a result of research done since the model was first developed.

Formulation applies chiefly to deductive reasoning.  It describes the extent to which the reasoner is aware of her or his own reasoning.  It also includes reasoning that is automatic (e.g., the use of algebra) or superficial (i.e., it has the appropriate form, but the underlying understanding is absent). 

Formality applies only to written deductive proofs, and categorises them according to their adherence to mathematical norms for the presentation of reasoning.  

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-6

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


These categories are linked by paths which indicate combinations which have been observed in past research.  Figure 1 (from Reid, 1995a) shows the model as it was first proposed.  Table 1 lists subtypes of the need to explain derived from work at the grade 10 research site. Section III contains additions based on the research reported here.

 Figure 1: The Model presented in Reid 1995

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-7

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section



 

 

E: Explaining to someone else

EH: Explaining how

EHT: Explaining how prompted by a teacher or written prompt

EHN: Explaining how not prompted by a teacher or written prompt.

EW: Explaining why

EWP: Explaining to someone else (spontaneously)

EWT: Explaining to someone else (in response to a teacher or written prompt question)

EWS: Explaining to someone else (in response to a non teacher or written prompt question)

EWC: Explaining as part of social activity in a community where explaining is a social norm, or part of the community’s emotional orientation.

U: Attempting to come to a personal understanding (explaining to oneself)

UM: Understanding the meaning of something (e.g., a definition)

UMT: Prompted by a teacher or written prompt

UMS: Not prompted by a teacher or written prompt.

UW: Understanding why something is the case (e.g., a theorem and it's proof)

UWT: Prompted by a teacher or written prompt

UWS: Not prompted by a teacher or written prompt.

 

Table 1: Revisions to the model based on work at the grade 10 site.  

Classroom research

While some research has been done in classrooms on the development of students’ reasoning, none has specifically addressed the issue of the development of deductive reasoning at both the primary and secondary levels or the role of students’ needs in this development.  Furthermore, much of the research that has focused on deductive reasoning is not directly applicable to contemporary Canadian schools as it has been done in contexts far removed from contemporary 

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-8

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


Canadian classrooms, and has employed models of deductive reasoning that neglect needs for reasoning. 

One example is Fawcett’s (1938) research which analysed the development of students’ reasoning in geometry and in real world situations during an experimental full year course in Euclidean geometry.  While many of Fawcett’s experimental methods are still useful, the curricular shift away from Euclidean geometry means that his findings are no longer directly applicable.  Balacheff, Arsac, Mante, and others (Arsac, Balacheff, & Mante, 1992; Arsac, Chapiron, Colonna, Germain, Guichard, & Mante, 1992) engaged in classroom research in France in which the development of mathematical reasoning was observed in the course of a unit on mathematical proof that is first introduced in the equivalent of grade 7.  As Canadian curricula do not provide such opportunities to focus directly on mathematical reasoning, this research is also not directly applicable, however some of the methods used by these researchers are. 

Some research employing methods similar to those of the PRISM project has been done in primary schools.  Teacher/researchers have created classroom situations in which deductive reasoning might be expected to flourish, but their research focus has been on other issues. Lampert (1990) and Maher and Martino (1996) have conducted classroom research with grade 5 students on the development of mathematical communication and community that offers occasional hints at the reasoning of the students involved.  Zack (1995, 1997; Graves & Zack, 1996) is also engaged in research at the grade 5 level, focused on children’s construction of mathematical knowledge through problem solving and joint activity.  She is a collaborator in the PRISM project.

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-9

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


Theoretical and methodological background.

The PRISM project derives both its psychological perspective and its general methodological structure from enactivist theories of cognition, as described by Maturana and Varela (Maturana & Varela, 1992; Varela, Thompson & Rosch, 1991).  Enactivism as a theory of cognition acknowledges the importance of the individual in the construction of a lived world, but emphasizes that the structure of the individual coemerges with this world in the course of, and as a requirement for, the continuing inter-action of the individual and the situation.  From this perspective the role of reasoning in mathematics is seen not as part of mathematics, nor as a result of the students’ needs in a situation, but as a part of the inter-action between them.  Similarly the data and interpretations of enactivist research are seen as coming out of the inter-actions between students, teachers, teaching materials, and the research context.  This approach captures much of the richness of classroom activity, a richness that is difficult to capture in other ways.

This enactivist style of inquiry (described further in Kieren, Gordon Calvert, Reid & Simmt, 1996, 1995) has also been described as “brico-logical” (Reid 1996, 1995a).   Brico-logical research combines the flexibility and creativity of bricolage with an underlying logic of inquiry.  Bricolage, as it is used in conceptualizing brico-logical research, favours the production of complex structures, theories, models, etc. appropriate to research on complex systems such as human learners.  The logic of the brico-logical methodology comes from the questions chosen for research, and the theories and models with which the research begins.  In practice this involves a continuous and continuing reinterpretation of data from multiple perspectives that are themselves being continually reinterpreted by the data. 

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-10

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


In the PRISM project these multiple perspectives are provided both through the active involvement of a number of researchers, and through the use of multiple sites for research.  The cooperating teachers, research assistants, collaborator and principal investigator each bring to the research unique perspectives and goals.  The backgrounds of the teachers and researchers involved are such that their goals include developing better teaching methods, exploring classroom social structures, and investigating learning of particular concepts.  The interaction of investigations with different goals and perspectives allows for the creation of richer research contexts, encouraging the development of deeper understandings of all the questions brought to the situation.  The presence of multiple perspectives at each research site corresponds to the presence of the principal investigator’s perspective at multiple sites.  The same complexity is offered by the multiple data sources in different classrooms, at different grade levels, over several years.

Method

At the Grade 2 site data was collected by observing groups of students working with a research assistant at learning stations.  Several learning stations involving game playing and other mathematical activities (See Section II and Appendix A)  were developed to provide opportunities for students to reason deductively and in other ways.  The children's mathematical activity was video taped and audio taped.  Data was collected primarily with research questions 2 and 3 in mind, but also for comparison with the existing data from the grade 5 and grade 10 sites.  A revised model (produced in the grade 10 research) was used to analyse the data.  

The Psychology of Students' Reasoning in School Mathematics
David A Reid
Page I-11

To Beginning of Section | To End of Section


previous     table of contents     next